Findings


1 Remarks

1.1 General remarks

  • Unpredictable influence:
    • Observations WILD: [1].
  • Competition:
    • Observations WILD: competition with native species [2].
    • FARM: ADULTS in offshore cages showed signs of spawning and were observed to release gametes into open sea, increasing competition with native conspecifics [3].
    • FARM/WILD: ADULTS released from a fish farm in a simulated escape used the same habitat and caught the same prey as native conspecifics [4].  
  • Disease transmission: no data found yet.
  • Interbreeding:
    • Observations WILD: breeding with native populations [5] [6] [7] could reduce fitness and productivity in hybrids (because of the smaller genetic variability in farmed individuals) and eventually survival [8].
[]
  • FARM: majority of released ADULTS were found on seagrass bottom, some also on 1) fine sandy bottoms, 2) a mixture of seaweed, seagrass, sandy, and rocky bottom, and 3) detritus and gravel bottoms [4].
[]

1.2 Other remarks

No data found yet.


2 Ethograms

[]
[]

3 Distribution

  • Species occurrence (natural and introduced). Note: areas either verified by FAO records ("good" point) or not [25].
  • Observations eastern Atlantic: [11] [26]. Bay of Cadiz, Atlantic, southern Spanish coast [27], Bay of Machico and Ribeira Brava, Atlantic, Madeira [2], Ria Formosa, southern Portuguese coast [10], Wexford Harbour, Irish Sea basin, Ireland [28].
  • Observations Mediterranean: Bardawil lagoon, Mediterranean, Egypt [29], Beymelek lagoon, Mediterranean, southwestern Turkish coast [30], Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [31] [32], Mar Menor, Mediterranean, southern Spanish coast [33], Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34], Messolonghi-Etoliko lagoon, Mediterranean [35], Mirna Estuary, Adriatic Sea, Croatia [36], Gulf of Olbia, Mediterranean, Sardinia [37], two lagoons on Thyrrenian coast, Mediterranean, Italy [38], Venice lagoon, northern Adriatic Sea, Italy [39].
[]
  • Observations eastern Atlantic: Atlantic ocean, southern Madeiran coast [2].
  • Observations Pacific ocean: Gulf of California, Mexico [1].
[]

4 Natural co-existence

No data found yet.


5 Substrate and/or shelter

5.1 Substrate

  • Plants: has been reported from seagrass beds:
    • Observations WILD: [11], Messolonghi-Etoliko lagoon, Mediterranean [35].
    • WILD: preferred seagrass over 1) patches where seagrass had been removed or 2) unvegetated coarse-sandy substrate [37].
  • Rocks and stones: has been reported from rocky bottoms:
    • Observations WILD: Bay of Machico and Ribeira Brava, Atlantic, Madeira [2].
    • LAB: JUVENILES touched tank bottom with open mouth and closed mouth afterwards, grabbed or chewed gravel before ejecting it [40].
  • Sand and mud: has been reported from salt marsh creeks and muddy bottoms:
    • Observations salt marsh creeks WILD: Bay of Cadiz, Atlantic, southern Spanish coast [27], Venice lagoon, northern Adriatic Sea, Italy [39], Ria Formosa, southern Portuguese coast [10], Mar Menor, Mediterranean, southern Spanish coast [33].
    • Observations muddy bottoms WILD: Venice lagoon, northern Adriatic Sea, Italy [39], Ria Formosa, southern Portuguese coast [10], Wexford Harbour, Irish Sea basin, Ireland [28], Beymelek lagoon, Mediterranean, southwestern Turkish coast [30], Mar Menor, Mediterranean, southern Spanish coast [33].
  • Other substrate: no data found yet.
[]

5.2 Shelter or cover

  • Plants: no data found yet.
  • Rocks and stones: no data found yet.
  • Sand and mud:
    • WILD: ADULTS might bury themselves in sand during night [10].
  • Other cover: no data found yet.
[]

6 Food, foraging, hunting, feeding

6.1 Trophic level and general considerations on food needs

  • Observations: 3.7±0.0 se [41].
[]
  • Mainly carnivorous [F1]. The fishery that provides fish meal and fish oil has two major impacts:
    1. It contributes considerably to overfishing, as it accounts for 1/4 [42] or even 1/3 [43] of the world catch volume.
    2. It challenges animal welfare, because in the face of 450-1,000 MILLIARD wild fishes caught worldwide each year to be processed into fish meal or fish oil [44], the individual fish gets overlooked and, thus, suffering increases at rearing, live marketing, and slaughtering levels [45].
[]

6.2 Food items

  • Food items: carnivorous:
    • Observations WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: [11].
  • Food items and habitat: no data found yet.
  • Food items and life stages: crustacean larvae or Nematoda as JUVENILES, Bivalvia and Gastropoda as ADULTS:
    • WILD: JUVENILES <30 mm mainly: Cirripedia larvae [48] or Nematoda [38], to a lesser extent: Amphipodia, Calanoida, Copepoda, Cyclopoida, Harapacticoida, and Polychaeta, only seldomly: Algae, Chironomidae larvae, Cladocera, Crustacea larvae, Echinodermata larvae, fish larvae, microalgae, Mysidacea, Ostracoda, plant detritus, and Rotatoria [48] [38].
    • WILD: JUVENILES <85 mm: Amphipoda, Mysidacea, plant detritus, and Polychaeta, to a lesser extent: Fish eggs and larvae and Harpacticoida, only seldomly: Anthozoa, Bivalvia, Calanoida, Copepoda, Cumacea, Cyclopoida, Gastropoda, Isopoda, microalgae, Nematoda, Ostracoda, and Ruppida maritime [48] [38].
    • WILDJUVENILES >70 mm mainly: Bivalvia and Gastropoda, to a lesser extent: Amphipoda, Carcinus aestuarii, Cumacea, Cymodocea nodosa, Irregularia, Isopoda, Ostracoda, Tanaidacea, and Polychaeta, and only seldomly: algae, Bryozoa, Crustacea, Decapoda, Mysidacea, Nematoda, pisces, Ruppia maritima, and Urochordata [49] [38] [4].
  • Food preference: no data found yet.
  • Food partitioning: no data found yet.
  • Prey density: no data found yet.
  • Prey size selectivity: no data found yet.
  • Particle size:
    • LAB: FRY <7 mm differed in food preferences between live and microencapsulated food, large and small prey. With microencapsulated food, they preferred smaller size compared to live food and soft shell [50].
[]
  • LAB: feeding FRY rotifers enriched with arachidonic acid (long-chain n-6 poly unsaturated fatty acid) prior to stress increased stress tolerance and survival [51].
  • LAB: supplementation with the probiotic Pdp11 (bacteria Shewanella putrefaciens) improved stress tolerance in JUVENILES to high stocking density of 30 kg/m3 by attenuating the increase in the plasma cortisol level [52].
  • LAB: a diet lacking vitamin E decreased stress resistance and survival in JUVENILES [53].
[]

6.3 Feeding behaviour

  • WILD: individuals grabbed prey by partially burying their heads into the substrate [9].
[]
  • FARM: in semi-submerged cages at stocking density of 3 kg/m3, individuals swam predominantly horizontally, towards the bottom, and with frequent turns, resembling random searching for food in the wild [54].
  • LAB: feeding at random times results in arrhythmic pattern and constantly high frequency of JUVENILES and ADULTS locomotor activity [55] [56] as well as high plasma cortisol levels indicating stress in ADULTS [56]. Also, JUVENILES had a twofold higher level of blood glucose after feeding than scheduled-fed JUVENILES, suggesting poor regulation of blood glucose [55]. Scheduled feeding, on the other hand, increased locomotor activity in JUVENILES [55] and ADULTS [56] and amylase and alkaline protease in ADULTS [56] some hours before mealtime, thereby anticipating feeding and optimizing food intake and nutrient use.
[]
  • LAB, JUVENILES: high feed delivery rate increased the chewing rate, but relatively higher waste from chewing resulted in a worse, i.e. higher, FOOD CONVERSION RATIO than a slow delivery rate. To determine a proper feed delivery rate, pellet sinking rate and pellet size has to be taken into account and whether individuals may collect feed from the bottom of a tank or it is lost outside a sea cage [57].
[]
  • LAB, JUVENILES: rations of 2 and 2.5% body weight increased swimming speed and frequency of sharper angled turns – indicating higher competition – during meals compared with higher rations. Ration of 3.5% body weight resulted in lower feeding intensity, higher waste, and therefore lower feeding efficiency. Ration of 3% body weight benefited growth compared to lower rations and reduced waste compared to higher rations [57].
[]
  • LAB: competition for food works as a social mechanism regulating growth in captivity with ADULTS [20] and JUVENILES [21]: when food was limited and defensible, dominance hierarchies set in, ADULTS grew better when surrounded by smaller ADULTS than when surrounded by larger ADULTS [20]. Dominant JUVENILES had a higher relative specific growth rate [21] and a lower FOOD CONVERSION RATIO [22] than subordinate JUVENILES. Subordinate JUVENILES had lower plasma cortisol levels [58] and experienced immunosuppression [22].
  • LAB: when food was unlimited, ADULTS surrounded by larger companions grew faster than when surrounded by smaller companions, probably to level out size differences in fish groups and avoid standing out in the eyes of predators [20].
[]
  • Feeding and low temperatures: ceases ingestion below 12 °C:
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS decreased ingestion below 16 °C [49] and ceased ingestion below 12 °C [34].
    • LAB: ceased ingestion below 12 °C [59] [60].
  • Feedings and high temperatures:
    • WILD: JUVENILES ceased ingestion above 30 °C [61].
[]
  • For feeding and...
    ...vision  [F2],
    ...olfaction  [F3],
    ...establishing hierarchy  [F4],
    ...dominance  [F5].

7 Photoperiod

7.1 Daily rhythm

  • Daily rhythm:
    • LAB, ADULTS: in the cold season (<20 °C, 9-11 h photoperiod), peak around dusk, when temperatures increase [14] [15]. In the warm season (>20 °C, 12-14 h photoperiod), either peaks after dawn and before dusk [14] or mid-day peak [15].
  • Nocturnal activity and aggregation type:
    • WILD: larger ADULTS more active during night, probably because isolated. Smaller ADULTS more active during day, probably in a school [10].
    • LAB: ADULTS did not completely abandon swimming during resting periods, but were measurably active. In a school with 600 others, higher swimming activity during the day than at night, whereas in a group with three others or isolated, swimming activity highest at night [12].
  • Phototaxis: photonegative:
    • LAB: newly hatched LARVAE benefited in first 60+ hours until mouth opening from dark surrounding: slower absorption of oil globule and yolk sac, higher survival rate without illumination in contrast to 24 hours 450 lux (results for 24 hours 30 lux in between), probably because LARVAE do not move as much and do not spend as much energy [62].
  • For daily rhythm and depth  [F6].
[]
  • LAB: LARVAE survived with higher probability and grew bigger under a photoperiod of 24 h than under 12 h [63].
  • LAB: alteration of light/dark – not of light intensity – seems decisive to affect ADULTS [12], but JUVENILES grew better under 200 lux than 80 lux [64].
  • LAB: keeping JUVENILES-ADULTS under a manipulated photoperiod matching the longest day of the year increased growth [65].
[]

7.2 Light intensity

No data found yet.

7.3 Light colour

  • LAB, JUVENILES: red light slightly increased brain dopaminergic activity, indicating stress, and slightly decreased growth rate and food efficiency [66].
[]

8 Water parameters

8.1 Water temperature

  • Standard temperature range:
    • Observations WILD: 8-30 °C (ADULTS leave October-November): two lagoons on Thyrrenian coast, Mediterranean, Italy [38], 11-30 °C: Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34] [34]; 2-30 °C (JUVENILES leave November-February): Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [32], 13-30 °C: Beymelek lagoon, Mediterranean, southwestern Turkish coast [30], 11.2-30 °C: Mar Menor, Mediterranean, Southern Spanish coast [33].
  • Temperature preference: no data found yet.
  • Migration temperature:
    • Spring through autumn, FRY and ADULTS migrate to coastal lagoons when temperatures are 11-30 °C and leave to the open sea when temperatures are below that range Standard temperature range, [F7].
  • For temperature and swimming speed  [F8].
[]
  • Lower and upper lethal limits:
    • LABLARVAE reared at 16-22 °C were healthy and survived but showed abnormalities and short survival below and above this temperature range. At 12 °C and 30 °C, LARVAE did not hatch at all [67].
    • LAB: survival decreased the longer LARVAE were reared in 24.5 °C water instead of 19 °C [23].
  • Low temperatures and winter syndrome:
    • FARM: of JUVENILES-ADULTS reared in the northern Mediterranean sea, some developed “winter syndrome” or “winter disease”, a pathology including lethargy, abnormal swimming, starvation, immunosuppression, and ultimately resulting in death [68] [69] [70].
    • The disease is multifactorial, though, low temperatures alone may not be sufficient:
[]
  • Temperature must exceed: no data found yet.
  • Temperature must not go beyond: no data found yet.
  • Optimal temperature for growth:
    • WILD/LABJUVENILES-ADULTS: better growth in Atlantic compared to Mediterranean due to less fluctuations in temperatures in the first [72].
    • FARM: 25 °C [73]-[61].
  • For temperature and feeding  [F9].
[]

8.2 Oxygen

No data found yet.

8.3 Salinity

  • Salinity tolerance:
    • Natural and introduced distribution in saltwater [F10] [F11], but adjusts to brackish water as well [F7] [F12].
  • Standard salinity range: no data found yet.
[]
  • Lower and upper lethal limits:
    • LAB: adaptability to different salinities increases with age. More than half of LARVAE three days post hatching survived in salinities from 10-25.5‰, whereas from 30 days post hatching on, LARVAE survived salinities of 5.1-45.1‰ [74].
  • Salinity change and stress: no data found yet.
[]
  • LAB: 3.5-fold higher survival, better swim bladder inflation, and higher weight of 30 day old FRY in 25‰ salinity than in 40‰ salinity [75].
  • LAB: higher growth of FRY under 18-28‰ than 8‰ or 38‰. FRY could adapt to larger range when gradually rather than abruptly introduced to certain salinity levels [76].
  • LAB: JUVENILES, acclimated to brackish water with a salinity of 12‰, grew better than under 6‰ or 38‰ salinities [77].
[]

8.4 pH

No data found yet.

8.5 Turbidity

No data found yet.

8.6 Water hardness

No data found yet.

8.7 NO4

No data found yet.

8.8 Other

No data found yet.


9 Swimming

9.1 Swimming type, swimming mode

  • Swimming type: carangiform:
    • Observations FARM[78].
  • Ontogenesis of swimming behaviour:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: changes with developmental stage and environment: streamline body shape at early metamorphosis (<20 mm) more appropriate for fast swimming in pelagic larval niche; deep and relatively laterally compressed body shape at later metamorphosis (>25 mm) more suited for manoeuvrability in demersal juvenile environment [17].
[]

9.2 Swimming speed

  • Absolute swimming speed:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: the absolute critical swimming speed increases with body length [16].
  • Relative swimming speed:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: the relative critical swimming speed decreases with body length [16] [17].
    • LAB: spontaneously swimming ADULTS do not swim faster than 0.5 body lengths/s, although the size of the respirometer may have an influence on the velocity. When forced to swim, ADULTS reach 3.5 body lengths/s [79].
  • Swimming speed and temperature:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: bell-shaped relationship between speed and temperature with the maximum relative critical swimming speed at 25 °C [17].
[]

9.3 Home range

  • WILD: ADULTS released at capture site moved little, whereas those released 4 km away moved back to the capture site [10].
  • WILD: otoliths pointed to use of various lagoons [32].
  • FARM: majority of ADULTS stayed within radius of 800 m around release site for first five days. Food pellets in stomachs even two months after release indicated site fidelity in some ADULTS [4].
[]

9.4 Depth

  • Depth range in the wild:
    • Observations ≤3 m WILD: 0-2 m: Gulf of Olbia, Mediterranean, Sardinia [37], 0.9-3 m: two lagoons on Thyrrenian coast, Mediterranean, Italy [38], 1±0.3 m: Venice lagoon, northern Adriatic Sea, Italy [39], average 0.8 m: Messolonghi-Etoliko lagoon, Mediterranean, Greece [61], 0.5-3 m: Bardawil lagoon, Mediterranean, Egypt [29].
    • Observations ≤5 m WILD: average 3.5 m: Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34], 0.8-4 m: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [32], 0.5-5 m: Beymelek lagoon, Mediterranean, southwestern Turkish coast [30], average 3.6 m: Mar Menor, Mediterranean, southern Spanish coast [33].
    • Observations ≤30 m WILD: <30 m [11]; 15-30 m: Mirna Estuary, Adriatic Sea, Croatia [36], 10-12 m: Bay of Machico and Ribeira Brava, Atlantic, Madeira [2].
    • WILD: seldomly until 150 m, depending on geographical region [11]  [F10] [F11].
  • Depth in cages or tanks: no data found yet.
  • Depth preference: no data found yet.
  • Depth and daily rhythm:
    • FARM, ADULTS: after planned release, greater swimming depth during morning hours (7-17 m) compared to night time (1-5 m), but might be due to farm feeding at 6 a.m.. Escapees could have fed on waste feed beneath cages [4].
  • Depth and low temperatures: no data found yet.
  • Depth and high temperatures: no data found yet.
  • Position in habitat and age: no data found yet.
  • Depth and light intensity: no data found yet.
  • Depth and noise: no data found yet.
  • Depth and threat: no data found yet.
[]

9.5 Migration

  • LARVAE hatch in the open sea. FRY move to coastal lagoons or estuaries where they remain as JUVENILES and ADULTS most of the year [F12]:
  • JUVENILES-ADULTS return to the sea in winter, probably due to sensitivity to temperatures [F13].
[]

10 Growth

10.1 Ontogenetic development

  • Observations time from fertilisation until hatching: no data found yet.
  • Observations size: ca 1 mm [81], 0.84-0.99 mm [82]FARM: 0.94-0.98 mm [83].
  • Observations weight: no data found yet.
[]
  • Observations age at yolk sac absorption and mouth opening FARM: day 4 [84].
  • Observations age at yolk sac absorption and mouth opening LAB: day 2-5 [85], day 4-5 [86], day 3 [62], day 4 [87], day 2-3 [88].
  • Development goes alongside pigmentation of the eye (FARM [84]), formation of the digestive tract (FARM [84]​), inflation of the swim bladder (LAB [88]), and improvement of swimming abilities (LAB [89]).
  • Observations TOTAL LENGTH LAB: 2.5 mm [89], 2.7 mm [62].
  • Observations weight: no data found yet.
[]
  • Observations age at beginning of exogenous feeding LAB: day 4-6 [89], day 5 [88].
  • Observations age and TOTAL LENGTH LAB: 5.5-7.5 mm, day 28: notochord started to flex; 10.5 mm, day 40: fin ray formation; 13 mm, day 70: body pigmentation started to show; 25 mm, day 100: squamation started; 25-71.5 mm, day 100-160: scale cover complete [88].
  • Observations weight: no data found yet.
[]
  • Observations age: [F14].
  • Observations age, TOTAL LENGTH, and weight WILD: 17-25 cm: two lagoons, Mediterranean, western Italian coast [80], 0-1 year, 2-25.2 cm: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [31], 11.8-20.1 cm: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [32], 10.6-26 cm, 0.02-0.3 kg: Beymelek lagoon, Mediterranean, southwestern Turkish coast [30].
  • Sexual maturity for 50% of JUVENILES: first or second year of life, 20.5-32.6 cm:
    • Observations age and TOTAL LENGTH WILD: end of year 1, 27.6±1.34 cm: Banyuls-sur-Mer, Mediterranean, France [91], 18 months, 32.6 cm: Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34],1.67 years, 25.8 cm: Port Said, southeastern Mediterranean, Egypt [92], 0.47 years, 20.5 cm for males and 0.83 years, 22.8 cm for females: Bardawil lagoon, Mediterranean, Egypt [29], 24 cm: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [31]
[]
  • Maturation and photoperiod manipulation:
    • LAB: keeping JUVENILES under a manipulated photoperiod matching the longest day of the year postponed gonadal development and reproduction [93] [65] and increased growth [65].
  • Maturation and temperature manipulation: no data found yet.
[]
  • Observations age, TOTAL LENGTH, and weight WILD: 0-1 years, 20.5-32.6 cm [F15], 70 cm [11], 5 years and average 46.7 cm: Mediterranean, off Alexandrian coast, Egypt [94], 68 cm: Atlantic, off Sines and Sagres coast, Portugal [95], 12 years, 57.5 cm, 2.5 kg: Mirna estuary, northern Adriatic, Croatia [36], 44.4 cm: Ria Formosa, Algarve, Portugal [49], 3-7 years, average 44.2-54.9 cm, 1.5-2 kg, maximum 61 cm, 3.4 kg: Mellah lagoon, Algeria [34], 6 years, 50.8 cm: Gulf of Lions, northwest Mediterranean, France [31], 4 years, 26.3-39 cm: Gulf of Lions, northwest Mediterranean, France [32].
[]

10.2 Sexual conversion

  • Protandric hermaphrodite: JUVENILES develop first into males at 2-3 years, 10-30 cm, then turn into females at 2-3 years, 15-45 cm:
    • Observations males: 2 years, 20-30 cm [82], 2-3 years, 20-30 cm [9]; WILD: 22 cm: Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34], 10-17 cm: Port Said, southeastern Mediterranean, Egypt [92], 22 cm: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [31].
    • Observations females: 2-3 years [11], 2-3 years or 33-40 cm [82], 35-40 cm [9]; WILD: 43-45 cm: Mellah lagoon, Mediterranean, Algeria [34], 15-27 cm: Port Said, southeastern Mediterranean, Egypt [92], 28 cm: Gulf of Lions, Mediterranean, France [31].
  • Ambisexual gonad = can develop into testis or ovary (ovotestis) [91].
  • Terminal sex: female sex is terminal sex: males can continue as males or convert to females, females cannot revert (e.g. [96]).
  • Conversion from male to female:
    • In males, after spawning  [F16], ca May-August, decreasing testicular and increasing ovarian portion form ambisexual gonad. The testicular portion remains latent, the ovarian portion grows, oogonia differentiate follicles [97].
    • At ca September+, gonads recognizably develop into ovaries (future females) or testes (future males) and grow [97]. For more details  [96] [91].
[]
  • Sex and temperature manipulation: no data found yet.
  • Sex and hormone treatment: no data found yet.
  • Sex and genetic manipulation: no data found yet.
  • Sex and other manipulation: for sex and manipulation and male:female ratio  [F17].
[]

10.3 Sex ratio

No data found yet.

10.4 Effects on growth

  • Growth: halts during annulus formation [94] which takes place November-March, as revealed by scale readings and otolith investigations:
    • Observations scale readings WILD: December-February [94], November [34].
    • Observations otolith investigations WILD: January [92], November-March [31], December [32].
  • Natural growth rate: 100-200 g per year depending on environmental conditions:
  • Growth heritability:
    • LAB: weight and length are heritable [98].
[]
  • Growth and aquaculture system:
    • FARM: muscle tissue from JUVENILES reared in off-shore cages was comparable to that from wild-caught individuals [99].
  • Growth and music:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: better growth when exposed to music than in a control environment without music [64].
[]
  • For growth and...
    ...feed delivery  [F18],
    ...food competition  [F19],
    ...photoperiod  [F20],
    ...water temperature  [F21],
    ...salinity  [F22],
    ...stocking density  [F23].

10.5 Deformities and malformations

  • WILD/FARM: of wild-caught LARVAE, 31.9% with malformations (mild to serious); of hatchery-reared, 98.3-100%. Of wild-caught, 4.2% with at least one serious anomaly (kyphosis, lordosis, vertebrae fusion or deformation, splanchnocranium deformities); of hatchery-reared, 47.9-100% [100].
  • WILD/FARM: in 71-86.2% of hatchery-reared JUVENILES (intensive conditions), lateral line differed from that of wild-caught JUVENILES [101].
  • FARM: 5.4% of LARVAE developed body deformations: large head, compressed body, lordosis, kyphosis, kypholordosis, disoriented rays, frayed gills, notch in opercular cover, tumour in swim bladder; another 2.4% with mild kypholordosis [102].
  • FARM: 27% of LARVAE born with serious anomalies like axial deviations (e.g., lordosis), operculum atrophies, or cranial abnormalities [85].
  • FARM: of intensively reared LARVAE, 15.6-16.7% with opercular complex deformity [103].
  • FARM: of intensively reared LARVAE, 10% grew to ADULTS with 39 types of deformations: 17 affecting spinal column (lordosis, vertebral fusion), maxilar, operculum, jaw, and dorsal fin, 22 being different combinations of at least two of the above [104].
  • FARM: of LARVAE, 5.6% with lordosis, 7.9% with lacking operculum [105].
  • LAB: of 31 JUVENILES and ADULTS, 14 with lordosis (45.2%) [106]. More recent research needed to see if deformities continue to be a current issue.
[]
  • Bacteria:
    • LAB: higher frequency of bacteria detection in lordotic compared to normal individuals in swim bladder (50% versus 17.6%), brain (14.3% versus 0%), vertebral column (7.1% versus 0%), and liver (7.1% versus 0%). Authors suspect higher susceptibility to infection is not cause for but consequence of increased stress from abnormality [106].
  • Uninflated swim bladder:
    • LAB: among FRY with functional swim bladder no observed lordosis over 2 months. Of FRY without swim bladder, majority developed lordosis from ca 0.8 g on: more incidences of lordosis (90% versus 20%) under forced swimming compared to static water (20 cm/s water current versus <0.5 cm/s). Steeper lordotic angle (70° versus 30°) under forced swimming compared to static water. Deformities mostly in spine region affected by muscle pressure during swimming. Although swim bladder secondarily inflated in 100% of deformed individuals at 54 g body weight the latest, this did not correct deformity. Lordotic angle increased as body weight increased [107].
  • Missing natural selection in aquaculture environment:
    • LAB: suspected [85].
  • Physical stress:
    • WILD/FARM: differing lateral line of hatchery-reared JUVENILES compared to wild-caught JUVENILES not due to osteological malformations of spinal chord (skoliosis, lordosis, kyphosis) [101].
    • WILD/LAB: increase in frequency of serious anomalies from semi-intensive (47.9-54.5%) to intensive culture (69.2-100%) [100].
    • LAB: opercular complex deformity initially observed at 23 mm (not before), increasing frequency until day 65, maintaining frequency until day 100. Hint of physical stress as cause, because ossification of bones begins at size of around 6 mm. Other factors (nutritional, metabolic, behavioural) possible [103].
  • Inheritance:
    • LAB: high frequency (6.5%) of triple column abnormality (lordosis-scoliosis-kyphosis) in one family hints on polygenic origin [104]. Weak tendency of heritability for lacking operculum but not for lordosis – authors suggest to look for external parameters in aquaculture for explanation [105].
[]

11 Reproduction

11.1 Nest building

  • Nest building and substrate: no data found yet.
  • Nest building and water velocity: no data found yet.
  • Nest building and water depth: no data found yet.
  • Nest building: no data found yet.
  • For breeding type  [F12].
[]

11.2 Attraction, courtship, mating

  • Attraction and body colour:
    • LAB, ADULTS: beginning in July, the pigmentation intensity of the yellow belly strip, orange operculum spot, and yellow marking behind the pelvic fin increased every month until October. Whereas the colours faded for males after October, they remained unchanged for females through spawning. In general, females have more intense orange operculum colour and yellow belly strip colour than males. The role of the colouring in courtship or attraction is unclear [108].
[]

11.3 Spawning

  • Spawning substrate: no data found yet.
  • Spawning season: October-March, but can extend to May or June depending on environmental factors [81]:
    • Observations WILD: October-December [11], November-December [91], December [34], November-February, peaking in December [92].
    • Observations FARM: until May [81], until June [83].
    • Observations LAB: December-January [109], December-March [97], February [110], peaking in February [108].
  • Spawning (day)time: no data found yet.
  • Spawning temperature: for lower and upper lethal limit for hatching [F24].
  • Spawning salinity: seawater  [F7].
  • Spawning and water velocity: no data found yet.
  • Spawning depth: no data found yet.
  • Spawning density: no data found yet.
[]
  • Male:female ratio resulting in spawning: no data found yet.
  • Composition of broodstock:
    • FARM: in small groups of ADULTS, grouped females fluctuated less in the number of eggs than single females, and more eggs were fertilised by the males [111].
    • FARM, ADULTS: females spawn in large groups or schools, so minimally 5-7 females should be kept together to reduce stress and induce spawning in artificial environments [112]-[83].
    • Adding young individuals caused older males to develop into females; adding females caused older males to stay males [81].
    • LAB, ADULTS: when spermiating males were removed from small groups of mixed sex shortly before spawning, the number of released eggs decreased, vitellogenic oocytes underwent atresia, and ovulatory oocytes degenerated. Separation from the females did not affect spermiation by the males, though [113].
[]
  • Spawning sequence: no data found yet.
  • Spawning duration:
    • WILD/LAB, ADULTS: females injected with human chorionic gonadotropic spawned 10-30 min [114]. For frequency  [F25].
[]

11.4 Fecundity

  • Number of spawns:
    • WILD/LAB, ADULTS: wild-caught females stocked individually with two mature males and injected with low (100-400 i.u./kg body weigth) or high dose (up to 1,200 i.u./kg body weigth) dose of human chorionic gonadotropin. More females spawned four successive days or more when injected low dose of human chorionic gonadotropic than control group (17/40 versus 5/50); 14 low-dose females spawned on 1-3 days; nine low-dose females and 45 control females did not spawn. Spawning females continued spawning every 24 h over 4-100 days [114].
  • Fecundity per spawn:
    • Observations absolute fecundity WILD, ADULTS: 20,000-80,000 eggs per day [82].
    • WILD/LAB, ADULTS: wild-caught females injected with 100-400 i.u./kg body weight human chorionic gonadotropin: ca 4,200-41,000 eggs/spawn [114].
    • Observations relative fecundity WILD, ADULTS: 0.5-2 times females' body weight in eggs [81].
[]
  • Fecundity and lunar cycle:
    • FARM, ADULTS: the sum of eggs recorded during full moon almost doubled that recorded during new moon. Egg sum peaked on the first full moon day every month [115].
[]
  • Fecundity and temperature manipulation:
    • LAB, ADULTS: increasing the temperature from 14 °C to 18 °C induced 25% of females to release eggs [111].
  • Fecundity and hormone treatment:
    • Injecting females carrying oocytes in the last stages of vitellogenesis with human chorionic gonadotropin induced spawning (LAB [109] [114]), but mammalian gonadotropin-releasing hormone analogue (GnRHa) is as effective and more advantageous, for example by stimulating the female's own gonadotropin hormone [81].
    • GnRHa was – at low doses – more effective in inducing gonadotropin hormone release and ovulation in females than the more expensive salmon GnRHa [116]-[81].
    • GnRHa implants or microspheres prolonged the effect of a single injection and induced a highly predictable spawning pattern in over 80% of the treated females (as compared with 25% induced by a single injection) for periods ranging up to four months [81]. Compared to natural spawning, GnRHa delivery systems produced similar or even higher number and viability of eggs (LAB [111]) and hatching and survival rates of LARVAE [81].
[]

11.5 Brood care, breeding

  • Breeding type: sea spawner:
    • Observations:  [F7].
  • Nursery grounds:
[]

12 Senses

12.1 Vision

  • Vision and foraging:
    • LAB: 20 day old FRY increased ingestion of a microdiet by 50-60% when given visual access to Artemia nauplii compared to absence of Artemia nauplii [118].
[]
  • LAB, JUVENILES: more interactions with the bottom in the first week in tanks with blue and red-brown glass gravel than green or no glass gravel. No difference in aggressive acts in tanks with blue and red-brown glass gravel, but higher aggression with green and highest aggression without glass gravel [64].
[]

12.2 Olfaction (and taste, if present)

  • LAB, ADULTS: rarely responsive to steroids, bile acid, prostaglandins. More responsive to amino acids, especially short-chain neutral and basic amino acids, and urine. Even more responsive to conspecific seminal and egg fluid. Highest amplitude of response to intestinal fluid of conspecifics, because probably a source of pheromones [119].
[]
  • Olfaction and foraging:
    • LAB: 20 day old FRY increased ingestion of a microdiet by 50-60% in the presence of filtrate water from an Artemia nauplii culture tank. In combination with visual access to Artemia nauplii, ingestion rates increased to 120% compared to the absence of visual or chemical Artemia stimuli [118].
[]

12.3 Hearing

  • Hearing type:
    • LAB, ADULTS: hearing GENERALIST [64].
  • Hearing spectrum: no data found yet.
[]
  • Hearing and orientation:
    • LAB: acoustic stimuli prevented JUVENILES from swimming near the wall boundary layer of the tank [17].
[]
  • Inverse effect:
    • LAB: acoustic stimuli with frequency band of 0.1-1 kHz increased amount of movement and haematocrit levels in ADULTS, indicating stress [122].
  • Direct effect:
    • LAB: in an environment simulating off-shore noises, JUVENILES grew better and had lower stress indices than JUVENILES in an on-shore or control environment [123].
[]
  • For music and growth  [F27].

12.4 Touch, mechanical sensing

No data found yet.

12.5 Lateral line

  • Lateral line system and sensing water movement and vibrations:
    • Detects local water movements, so that individual perceives and localises prey, enemies, and sexual partners [124]-[101].
    • Detects surface and low frequency waves in the vicinity of the fish body, indirectly detects vibrations from sound waves [125]-[101].
  • Involved body parts:
    • Neuromast receptor located in the scale is connected to the lateral line nerve [126]-[101].
    • Body lateral line contains 73-85 scales [127]-[101].
[]

12.6 Electrical sensing

No data found yet.

12.7 Nociception, pain sensing

No data found yet.

12.8 Other

No data found yet.


13 Communication

13.1 Visual

  • LAB: during an aggressive act, ADULTS [108] and JUVENILES [40] displayed darker body colouration, JUVENILES erected their dorsal fin [40].
[]

13.2 Chemical

No data found yet.

13.3 Acoustic

  • LAB: contracting sonic muscles excite the swim bladder they enclose which results in sounds like single knocking (percussion) and series of pulsed sounds (pulse trains) [18] [19].
[]

13.4 Mechanical

No data found yet.

13.5 Electrical

No data found yet.

13.6 Other

No data found yet.


14 Social behaviour

14.1 Spatial organisation

  • WILD: lived solitary or in small to large aggregations [11] [12] [13] [9], probably correlated with body length: larger individuals isolated, smaller ones in schools [10].
  • For aggregation type and daily rhythm  [F28].
[]
  • FARM, ADULTS: stocking density of >70 kg/m3 resulted in vigorous movement and earlier onset and resolution of rigour mortis [128]. Density of control condition was 30 kg/m3, so level at which stress commences could be <70 kg/m3 – further research needed.
  • LAB: stocking density of >22 kg/m3 resulted in increased plasma cortisol levels [129] [130] [131] [132] and decreased immune parameters [129] [133] in JUVENILES and earlier onset and resolution of rigour mortis in ADULTS [134]. Density of control conditions was 3-10 kg/m3 for JUVENILES (7 kg/m3 [129], 4 kg/m3 [130], 3-10 kg/m3 [131], 9 kg/m3 [133], 7 kg/m3 [132]) and 17 kg/m3 for ADULTS [134], so level at which stress commences could be <22 kg/m3further research needed.
[]
  • LAB, JUVENILES: being able to swim freely and with a certain velocity positively affected weight gain [60].
[]

14.2 Social organisation

  • Hierarchy and group size: in small groups, JUVENILES establish linear dominance hierarchies:
    • Observations LAB, JUVENILES: between four individuals [21]; groups of two individuals: one dominant, one subordinate, groups of five: one dominant, two betas, two subordinates, groups of 10: two dominants, four betas, four subordinates [22].
    • LAB: in small groups, JUVENILES recognised each other or conspecifics with certain behaviour and based on that established dominance hierarchies [135]-[20]
    • LAB: in large groups, JUVENILES constantly adjusted position to conspecifics and lived in anonymity [20]
    • LAB: no dominance hierarchy established in a group of 75 JUVENILES [22].
  • Establishing hierarchy:
    • LAB: among groups of four JUVENILES, hierarchy was established via fighting. Most interactions occurred during feeding. Rank usually matched size. Hierarchy among mixed-size JUVENILES was established faster (day 4 versus day 10) than among same-size JUVENILES. Number of aggressive acts increased the longer the JUVENILES were held together [21].
  • For linear hierarchy in food competition and growth  [F19].
[]
  • Features of dominance:
    • LAB: dominant JUVENILES stayed in place where food accumulates, were highly mobile in that area, and showed more aggressive behaviour than subordinate JUVENILES [22] [21].
    • LAB: dominant JUVENILES had a higher cortisol level than beta and control JUVENILES but lower than subordinates [22].
[]
  • Features of subordination:
    • LAB: to avoid confrontations with higher-ranked individuals, subordinate JUVENILES displayed vertical swimming in the upper water level [21], hardly moved in the tank, and kept a position away from food [21] [22].
  • Hierarchy and stress:
    • LAB: subordinate JUVENILES displayed decreased immunological parameters and an increased cortisol level compared to dominant, beta, and control JUVENILES [22].
[]

14.3 Exploitation

No data found yet.

14.4 Facilitation

No data found yet.

14.5 Aggression

  • Size-matched pairs:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: in single-size groups, short interactions, predominantly front and rear attacks, retreats. Few longer-lasting interactions included repeated elements of mutual frontal threat head-to-head display, circling, chasing, and nipping. No difference in order of aggressive acts to mixed-size groups [21].
  • Non-matched pairs:
    • LAB: in mixed-size populations, larger FRY displayed aggression and cannibalism towards smaller FRY [23].
    • LAB: dominant JUVENILES chased and nipped subordinate JUVENILES [22].
[]
  • Aggression and sex:
    • LAB, ADULTS: no clear sex bias in aggressive behaviour [108].
[]
  • For aggression and...
    ...substrate colour  [F29],
    ...dominance  [F5],
    ...learning  [F30],
    ...coping styles [F31].

14.6 Territoriality

No data found yet.


15 Cognitive abilities

15.1 Learning

  • LAB: prior handling experience might increase aggression level, as JUVENILES with restraining experience exhibited more aggressive behaviour (lower latency to chase and higher number of chases) than control individuals [24].
[]

15.2 Memory

No data found yet.

15.3 Problem solving, creativity, planning, intelligence

No data found yet.

15.4 Other

No data found yet.


16 Personality, coping styles

  • Fighters and non-fighters:
    • LAB: after restraining experience, JUVENILES differed in cortisol levels (range: 6.2-117.3 ng/mL). When paired in dyads with naïve individuals for an aggressiveness test three months later, previously restrained JUVENILES varied in number of chases from 0 to 103. Fighters displayed lower cortisol levels than non-fighters, only accounting for 21% of the variation in aggressiveness, though. Further studies needed to find other reasons for different coping styles [24].
  • For aggressiveness and...
    ...establishing hierarchy [F4],
    ...dominance [F5],
    ...subordination [F32],
    ...size-grading [F33].

In the structure of menu item 16 and the definition of "AGGRESSIVENESS", we follow [136].

[]

17 Emotion-like states

17.1 Joy

No data found yet.

17.2 Relaxation

No data found yet.

17.3 Sadness

No data found yet.

17.4 Fear

No data found yet.


18 Self-concept, self-recognition

No data found yet.


19 Reactions to husbandry

19.1 Stereotypical and vacuum activities

No data found yet.

19.2 Acute stress

  • Handling and confinement: causes stress:
    • LAB, JUVENILES: handling and confinement caused stress, measured as increased plasma cortisol levels [132].
    • ADULTS: pre-slaughter handling and confinement caused stress, indicated by great activity and vigorous movements (FARM [128]), later death (FARM [128]), as well as earlier onset and earlier resolution of rigour mortis (FARM [128]; LAB [134]).
[]
  • For acute stress and...
    ...food competition  [F34],
    ...light colour  [F35],
    ...salinity  [F26],
    ...noise  [F36].

19.3 Chronic stress

  • For chronic stress and...
    ...feed enrichment  [F37],
    ...feed delivery  [F38],
    ...water temperature  [F24],
    ...noise  [F36]
    ...stocking density  [F39],
    ...dominance  [F5],
    ...subordination  [F32].

19.4 Stunning reactions

  • Stunning rules: to minimise pain reactions and enhance welfare before slaughter:
    1. induce insensibility as fast as possible,
    2. prevent recovery from stunning,
    3. monitor effectiveness (observations, neurophysiological measurements) [137].
[]
  • Pre-slaughter struggle time:
    a) percussive stunning: ADULTS: 0 min [138],
    b) electrical stunning: ADULTS: for at least 10 s with >200 mA: 0 min [138],
    c) immersion in ice-slurry: ADULTS: 5 min [138],
    d) immersion in chilled water: FARM, ADULTS: 25 min [128],
    e) asphyxia in air: 4-50 min:
    • Observations: FARM: 20-50 min [128]; 5 min at 0.1 °C, 5.5 min at 22 °C [137]; 4 min [138].
[]

Glossary

ADULTS = mature individuals, for details Findings 10.1 Ontogenetic development
AGGRESSIVENESS = agonistic reactions towards conspecifics. Tests: mirror image, social interaction/diadic encounters [136].
FARM = setting in farm environment
FOOD CONVERSION RATIO = (food offered / weight gained)
FRY = larvae from external feeding on, for details Findings 10.1 Ontogenetic development
GENERALIST = Generalists detect a narrow bandwidth of sound frequencies (<50-500 Hz, 1,500 Hz max.). High hearing threshold = cannot detect quieter sounds. Typically no swim bladder or no attachment of the swim bladder to the inner ear. Live in loud environments (rivers) [120] [121].
JUVENILES = fully developed but immature individuals, for details Findings 10.1 Ontogenetic development
LAB = setting in laboratory environment
LARVAE = hatching to mouth opening, for details Findings 10.1 Ontogenetic development
MILLIARD = 1,000,000,000 [46] [47]
TOTAL LENGTH = from snout to tip of caudal fin as compared to fork length (which measures from snout to fork of caudal fin) [90] or standard length (from head to base of tail fin) or body length (from the base of the eye notch to the posterior end of the telson)
WILD = setting in the wild


Bibliography

[1] Balart, Eduardo F., Juan Carlos Pérez-Urbiola, Lucía Campos-Dávila, Mario Monteforte, and Alfredo Ortega-Rubio. 2009. On the first record of a potentially harmful fish, Sparus aurata in the Gulf of California. Biological Invasions 11: 547–550. doi:10.1007/s10530-008-9269-3.
[2] Alves, Filipe M. A., and Catarina M. A. Alves. 2002. Two new records of seabreams (Pisces:Sparidae) from the Madeira Archipelago.
[3] Dimitriou, Evagelos, George Katselis, Dimitrios K Moutopoulos, Constantin Akovitiotis, and Constantin Koutsikopoulos. 2007. Possible influence of reared gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata, L.) on wild stocks in the area of the Messolonghi lagoon (Ionian Sea, Greece). Aquaculture Research 38: 398–408. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2109.2007.01681.x.
[4] Arechavala-Lopez, P., I. Uglem, D. Fernandez-Jover, J. T. Bayle-Sempere, and P. Sanchez-Jerez. 2012. Post-escape dispersion of farmed seabream (Sparus aurata L.) and recaptures by local fisheries in the Western Mediterranean Sea. Fisheries Research 121–122: 126–135. doi:10.1016/j.fishres.2012.02.003.
[5] Palma, J., J.a. Alarcon, C. Alvarez, E. Zouros, A. Magoulas, and J.p. Andrade. 2001. Developmental stability and genetic heterozygosity in wild and cultured stocks of gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 81: 283–288. doi:10.1017/S0025315401003757.
[6] Magoulas, Antonis. 2002. Genetic considerations in the introduction of aquacultured fish to natural ecosystems. In Aquaculture Challenges in Asia After the Bangkok Declaration on Sustainable Aquaculture The next step, 79–85. Beijing, China.
[7] Alarcón, J. A., A. Magoulas, T. Georgakopoulos, E. Zouros, and M. C. Alvarez. 2004. Genetic comparison of wild and cultivated European populations of the gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 230: 65–80. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(03)00434-4.
[8] Šegvić-Bubić, Tanja, Ivana Lepen, Željka Trumbić, Jelena Ljubković, Davorka Sutlović, Sanja Matić-Skoko, Leon Grubišić, Branko Glamuzina, and Ivona Mladineo. 2011. Population genetic structure of reared and wild gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata) in the Adriatic Sea inferred with microsatellite loci. Aquaculture 318: 309–315. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2011.06.007.
[9] Jobling, Malcolm, and Stefano Peruzzi. 2010. Seabreams and Porgies (Family: Sparidae). In Finfish Aquaculture Diversification, ed. Nathalie R. Le Francois, Malcolm Jobling, Chris Carter, and Pierre Blier, 361–373. CABI.
[10] Abecasis, David, and Karim Erzini. 2008. Site fidelity and movements of gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata) in a coastal lagoon (Ria Formosa, Portugal). Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 79: 758–763. doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2008.06.019.
[11] Bauchot, M.-L., J.-C. Hureau, and J. C. Miguel. 1981. Sparidae. In FAO species identification sheets for fishery purposes. Eastern Central Atlantic., ed. W. Fischer, G. Bianchi, and W. B. Scott. Vol. 4. Rome: FAO.
[12] Bégout, Marie-Laure, and Jean-Paul Lagardére. 1995. An acoustic telemetry study of seabream (Sparus aurata L.): first results on activity rhythm, effects of environmental variables and space utilization. Hydrobiologia 300–301: 417–423. doi:10.1007/BF00024483.
[13] Arabaci, Muhammed, Yasin Yilmaz, Saltuk Bugrahan Ceyhun, Orhan Erdogan, Hakan Galip Dorlay, Ibrahim Diler, Suleyman Akhan, et al. 2010. A Review on Population Characteristics of Gilthead Seabream (Sparus aurata). Journal of Animal and Veterinary Advances 9: 976–981. doi:10.3923/javaa.2010.976.981.
[14] Paspatis, M., D. Maragoudaki, and M. Kentouri. 2000. Self-feeding activity patterns in gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata), red porgy (Pagrus pagrus) and their reciprocal hybrids. Aquaculture 190: 389–401. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(00)00409-9.
[15] Velázquez, M., S. Zamora, and F. J. Martínez. 2004. Influence of environmental conditions on demand-feeding behaviour of gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Journal of Applied Ichthyology 20: 536–541. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0426.2004.00613.x.
[16] Basaran, Fatih, Huseyin Ozbilgin, and Yeliz Doganyilmaz Ozbilgin. 2007. Comparison of the swimming performance of farmed and wild gilthead sea bream, Sparus aurata. Aquaculture Research 38: 452–456. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2109.2007.01670.x.
[17] Koumoundouros, G., C. Ashton, G. Xenikoudakis, I. Giopanou, E. Georgakopoulou, and N. Stickland. 2009. Ontogenetic differentiation of swimming performance in Gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata, Linnaeus 1758) during metamorphosis. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 370: 75–81. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2008.12.001.
[18] Kouzoupis, S., and P. Papadakis. 2010. Modeling the swimbladder sound producing mechanism of the Gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). In . Istanbul.
[19] Kouzoupis, S., P. Papadakis, V. Boura, I. Xezonakis, and M. Kentouri. 2007. Preliminary investigation on sound production by two fish species: Sparus Aurata and Dicentrarchus Labrax. In Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics, 29:225–232. Holywell Park, Loughborough University, UK.
[20] Karplus, I., D. Popper, and O. Goldan. 2000. The effect of food competition and relative size of group members on growth of juvenile gilthead sea bream, Sparus aurata. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 22: 119–123. doi:10.1023/A:1007885525902.
[21] Goldan, Oded, Dan Popper, and IIan Karplus. 2003. Food Competition In Small Groups Of Juvenile Gilthead Sea Bream (Sparus Aurata). The Israeli Journal of Aquaculture - Bamidgeh 55: 94–106.
[22] Montero, D., G. Lalumera, M. S. Izquierdo, M. J. Caballero, M. Saroglia, and L. Tort. 2009. Establishment of dominance relationships in gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata juveniles during feeding: effects on feeding behaviour, feed utilization and fish health. Journal of Fish Biology 74: 790–805. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2008.02161.x.
[23] Tandler, A., M. Har’el, M. Wilks, A. Levinson, L. Brickell, S. Christie, E. Avital, and Y. Barr. 1989. Effect of environmental temperature on survival, growth and population structure in the mass rearing of the gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata. Aquaculture 78: 277–284. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(89)90105-1.
[24] Castanheira, Maria Filipa, Marcelino Herrera, Benjamín Costas, Luís E. C. Conceição, and Catarina I. M. Martins. 2013. Linking cortisol responsiveness and aggressive behaviour in gilthead seabream Sparus aurata: Indication of divergent coping styles. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 143: 75–81. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.11.008.
[25] Reviewed distribution maps for Gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). 2016. Aquamaps.
[26] Davis, P. S. 1988. Two occurrences of the gilthead, Sparus aurata Linnaeus 1758, on the coast of Northumberland, England. Journal of Fish Biology 33: 951–951. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1988.tb05545.x.
[27] Sánchez-Lamadrid, A. 2004. Effectiveness of releasing gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata, L.) for stock enhancement in the bay of Cádiz. Aquaculture 231: 135–148. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2003.08.015.
[28] Craig, G., D. Paynter, I. Coscia, and S. Mariani. 2008. Settlement of gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata L. in a southern Irish Sea coastal habitat. Journal of Fish Biology 72: 287–291. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01644.x.
[29] Ahmed, Mohamed S. 2011. Population dynamics and fisheries management of Gilthead sea bream, Sparus aurata (f. Sparidae) from Bardawil lagoon, North Sinai, Egypt. Egypt J. Aquat. Biol. Fish. 15: 57–69.
[30] Balik, Ismet, and Yilmaz Emre. 2013. Monthly variation in stock density and growth performance of juvenile gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata L., 1758) in Beymelek Lagoon, Antalya, Turkey. Pakistan J. Zool. 45: 687–693.
[31] Mercier, Lény, Jacques Panfili, Christelle Paillon, Awa N’diaye, David Mouillot, and Audrey M. Darnaude. 2011. Otolith reading and multi-model inference for improved estimation of age and growth in the gilthead seabream Sparus aurata (L.). Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 92: 534–545. doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2011.02.001.
[32] Mercier, Lény, David Mouillot, Olivier Bruguier, Laurent Vigliola, and Audrey M. Darnaude. 2012. Multi-element otolith fingerprints unravel sea−lagoon lifetime migrations of gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata. Marine Ecology Progress Series 444: 175–194. doi:10.3354/meps09444.
[33] Verdiell-Cubedo, David, Francisco J. Oliva-Paterna, Ana Ruiz-Navarro, and Mar Torralva. 2013. Assessing the nursery role for marine fish species in a hypersaline coastal lagoon (Mar Menor, Mediterranean Sea). Marine Biology Research 9: 739–748. doi:10.1080/17451000.2013.765580.
[34] Chaoui, Lamya, Mohamed Hichem Kara, and Jean Pierre Quignard. 2006. Growth and reproduction of the gilthead seabream Sparus aurata in Mellah lagoon (north-eastern Algeria). Scientia Marina 70: 545–552. doi:10.3989/scimar.2006.70n3545.
[35] Katselis, George, Constantin Koutsikopoulos, Evagelos Dimitriou, and Yiannis Rogdakis. 2003. Spatial patterns and temporal trends in the fisheries landings of the Messolonghi-Etoliko lagoons (Western Greek Coast). Scientia Marina 67. doi:10.3989/scimar.2003.67n4501.
[36] Kraljević, Miro, and Jakov Dulčić. 1997. Age and growth of gilt-head sea bream (Sparus aurata L.) in the Mirna estuary, northern Adriatic. Fisheries Research 31: 249–255. doi:10.1016/S0165-7836(97)00016-7.
[37] Guidetti, P., and S. Bussotti. 2002. Effects of seagrass canopy removal on fish in shallow Mediterranean seagrass (Cymodocea nodosa and Zostera noltii) meadows: a local-scale approach. Marine Biology 140: 445–453. doi:10.1007/s00227-001-0725-1.
[38] Tancioni, L., S. Mariani, A. Maccaroni, A. Mariani, F. Massa, M. Scardi, and S. Cataudella. 2003. Locality-specific variation in the feeding of Sparus aurata L.: evidence from two Mediterranean lagoon systems. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 57: 469–474. doi:10.1016/S0272-7714(02)00376-1.
[39] Franco, Anita, Piero Franzoi, Stefano Malavasi, Federico Riccato, Patrizia Torricelli, and Danilo Mainardi. 2006. Use of shallow water habitats by fish assemblages in a Mediterranean coastal lagoon. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 66: 67–83. doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2005.07.020.
[40] Batzina, Alkisti, and Nafsika Karakatsouli. 2012. The presence of substrate as a means of environmental enrichment in intensively reared gilthead seabream Sparus aurata: Growth and behavioral effects. Aquaculture 370–371: 54–60. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2012.10.005.
[41] Froese, R., and D. Pauly. 2014. FishBase. World Wide Web electronic publication. www.fishbase.org.
[42] Food and Agriculture Organization of the United nations. 2014. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.
[43] Watson, R., Jackie Alder, and Daniel Pauly. 2006. Fisheries for forage fish, 1950 to the present. In On the Multiple Uses of Forage Fish: from Ecosystems to Markets, ed. Jackie Alder and Daniel Pauly, 14:1–20. Fisheries Centre Research Reports 3. Vancouver, Canada: Fisheries Centre, University of British Columbia.
[44] Mood, A. 2012. Average annual fish capture for species mostly used for fishmeal (2005-2009). fishcount.org.uk.
[45] Mood, A., and P. Brooke. 2012. Estimating the Number of Farmed Fish Killed in Global Aquaculture Each Year.
[46] Kopf, Von Kristin. 2012. Milliarden vs. Billionen: Große Zahlen. Sprachlog. December 4.
[47] Weisstein, Eric W. 2018. Milliard. Text. MathWorld - a Wolfram Web resource. http://mathworld.wolfram.com/Milliard.html. Accessed February 2.
[48] Ferrari, I., and A. R. Chieregato. 1981. Feeding habits of juvenile stages of Sparus auratus L., Dicentrarchus labrax L. and Mugilidae in a brackish embayment of the Po River Delta. Aquaculture 25: 243–257. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(81)90186-1.
[49] Pita, C., S. Gamito, and K. Erzini. 2002. Feeding habits of the gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata) from the Ria Formosa (southern Portugal) as compared to the black seabream (Spondyliosoma cantharus) and the annular seabream (Diplodus annularis). Journal of Applied Ichthyology 18: 81–86. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0426.2002.00336.x.
[50] Fernández-Diaz, C., E. Pascual, and M. Yúfera. 1994. Feeding behaviour and prey size selection of gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata, larvae fed on inert and live food. Marine Biology 118: 323–328. doi:10.1007/BF00349800.
[51] Koven, W., Y. Barr, S. Lutzky, I. Ben-Atia, R. Weiss, M. Harel, P. Behrens, and A. Tandler. 2001. The effect of dietary arachidonic acid (20:4n−6) on growth, survival and resistance to handling stress in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata) larvae. Aquaculture 193: 107–122. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(00)00479-8.
[52] Varela, J. L., I. Ruiz-Jarabo, L. Vargas-Chacoff, S. Arijo, J. M. León-Rubio, I. García-Millán, M. P. Martín del Río, M. A. Moriñigo, and J. M. Mancera. 2010. Dietary administration of probiotic Pdp11 promotes growth and improves stress tolerance to high stocking density in gilthead seabream Sparus auratus. Aquaculture 309: 265–271. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2010.09.029.
[53] Montero, D., L. Tort, L. Robaina, J. M. Vergara, and M. S. Izquierdo. 2001. Low vitamin E in diet reduces stress resistance of gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata) juveniles. Fish Shellfish Immunology 11: 473–490. doi:10.1006/fsim.2000.0324.
[54] Sarà, G., A. Oliveri, G. Martino, S. Serra, G. Meloni, and A. Pais. 2010. Response of captive seabass and seabream as behavioural indicator in aquaculture. Italian Journal of Animal Science 6: 823–825. doi:10.4081/ijas.2007.1s.823.
[55] Montoya, A., J. F. López-Olmeda, M. Yúfera, M. J. Sánchez-Muros, and F. J. Sánchez-Vázquez. 2010. Feeding time synchronises daily rhythms of behaviour and digestive physiology in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 306: 315–321. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2010.06.023.
[56] Sánchez, J. A., J. F. López-Olmeda, B. Blanco-Vives, and F. J. Sánchez-Vázquez. 2009. Effects of feeding schedule on locomotor activity rhythms and stress response in sea bream. Physiology Behavior 98: 125–129. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.04.020.
[57] Andrew, J. E, J Holm, S Kadri, and F. A Huntingford. 2004. The effect of competition on the feeding efficiency and feed handling behaviour in gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.) held in tanks. Aquaculture 232: 317–331. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(03)00528-3.
[58] Cammarata, M., M. Vazzana, D. Accardi, and N. Parrinello. 2012. Seabream (Sparus aurata) long-term dominant-subordinate interplay affects phagocytosis by peritoneal cavity cells. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 26: 580–587. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.01.008.
[59] Tort, L., J. Rotllant, C. Liarte, L. Acerete, A. Hernández, S. Ceulemans, P. Coutteau, and F. Padros. 2004. Effects of temperature decrease on feeding rates, immune indicators and histopathological changes of gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata fed with an experimental diet. Aquaculture 229: 55–65. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(03)00403-4.
[60] Ibarz, A., M. Beltrán, J. Fernández-Borràs, M. A. Gallardo, J. Sánchez, and J. Blasco. 2007. Alterations in lipid metabolism and use of energy depots of gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata) at low temperatures. Aquaculture 262: 470–480. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2006.11.008.
[61] Katselis, George, Katerina Koukou, Evagelos Dimitriou, and Constantin Koutsikopoulos. 2007. Short-term seaward fish migration in the Messolonghi–Etoliko lagoons (Western Greek coast) in relation to climatic variables and the lunar cycle. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science 73: 571–582. doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2007.02.010.
[62] Saka, Sahin, Kürat Fırat, and Cüneyt Süzer. 2001. Effects Of Light Intensity On Early Life Development Of Gilthead Sea Bream Larvae (Sparus Aurata). Israeli Journal of Aquaculture - Bamidgeh 53: 139–146.
[63] Tandler, Amos, and Sarah Helps. 1985. The effects of photoperiod and water exchange rate on growth and survival of gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata, Linnaeus; Sparidae) from hatching to metamorphosis in mass rearing systems. Aquaculture 48: 71–82. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(85)90053-5.
[64] Papoutsoglou, S. E., N. Karakatsouli, A. Batzina, E. S. Papoutsoglou, and A. Tsopelakos. 2008. Effect of music stimulus on gilthead seabream Sparus aurata physiology under different light intensity in a re-circulating water system. Journal of Fish Biology 73: 980–1004. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2008.02001.x.
[65] Kissil, George Wm, Ingrid Lupatsch, Abigail Elizur, and Yonathan Zohar. 2001. Long photoperiod delayed spawning and increased somatic growth in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 200: 363–379. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(01)00527-0.
[66] Karakatsouli, Nafsika, Sofronios E. Papoutsoglou, Gianluca Pizzonia, Georgios Tsatsos, Aristeidis Tsopelakos, Stella Chadio, Dimitris Kalogiannis, Christina Dalla, Alexia Polissidis, and Zeta Papadopoulou-Daifoti. 2007. Effects of light spectrum on growth and physiological status of gilthead seabream Sparus aurata and rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss reared under recirculating system conditions. Aquacultural Engineering 36: 302–309. doi:10.1016/j.aquaeng.2007.01.005.
[67] Polo, A., M. Yúfera, and E. Pascual. 1991. Effects of temperature on egg and larval development of Sparus aurata L. Aquaculture 92: 367–375. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(91)90042-6.
[68] Gallardo, M. Ángeles, Mónica Sala-Rabanal, Antoni Ibarz, Francesc Padrós, Josefina Blasco, Jaume Fernández-Borràs, and Josep Sánchez. 2003. Functional alterations associated with “winter syndrome” in gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 223: 15–27. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(03)00164-9.
[69] TORT, L., F. PADRÓS, J. ROTLLANT, and S. CRESPO. 1998. Winter syndrome in the gilthead sea breamSparus aurata. Immunological and histopathological features. Fish Shellfish Immunology 8: 37–47. doi:10.1006/fsim.1997.0120.
[70] Tort, L, J Rotllant, and L Rovira. 1998. Immunological suppression in gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata of the North-West Mediterranean at low temperatures. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular Integrative Physiology 120: 175–179. doi:10.1016/S1095-6433(98)10027-2.
[71] Rotllant, J., P. H. M. Balm, S. E. Wendelaar-Bonga, J. Pérez-Sánchez, and L. Tort. 2000. A drop in ambient temperature results in a transient reduction of interrenal ACTH responsiveness in the gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata, L.). Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 23: 265–273. doi:10.1023/A:1007873811975.
[72] Hernández, Juan M, Eucario Gasca-Leyva, Carmelo J León, and J. M Vergara. 2003. A growth model for gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Ecological Modelling 165: 265–283. doi:10.1016/S0304-3800(03)00095-4.
[73] Bernabé, Gilbert. 1991. Acuicultura.
[74] Bodinier, Charlotte, Elliott Sucré, Laura Lecurieux-Belfond, Eva Blondeau-Bidet, and Guy Charmantier. 2010. Ontogeny of osmoregulation and salinity tolerance in the gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular Integrative Physiology 157: 220–228. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.06.185.
[75] Tandler, Amos, Fabio A. Anav, and Itzhak Choshniak. 1995. The effect of salinity on growth rate, survival and swimbladder inflation in gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata, larvae. Aquaculture 135: 343–353. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(95)01029-7.
[76] Klaoudatos, S D, and A J Conides. 1996. Growth, food conversion, maintenance and long-term survival of gilthead sea bream, Sparus auratus L., juveniles after abrupt transfer to low salinity. Aquaculture Research 27: 765–774. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2109.1996.t01-1-00792.x.
[77] Laiz-Carrión, Raúl, Susana Sangiao-Alvarellos, José M. Guzmán, María P. Martín del Río, José L. Soengas, and Juan M. Mancera. 2005. Growth performance of gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata in different osmotic conditions: Implications for osmoregulation and energy metabolism. Aquaculture 250: 849–861. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2005.05.021.
[78] Riera, Rodrigo, Víctor M. Tuset, Myriam Rodríguez, Óscar Monterroso, and Antoni Lombarte. 2017. Analyzing functional diversity to determine the effects of fish cages in insular coastal wild fish assemblages. Aquaculture 479: 384–395. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2017.06.014.
[79] Steinhausen, Maria Faldborg, John Fleng Steffensen, and Niels Gerner Andersen. 2010. The effects of swimming pattern on the energy use of gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata L.). Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 43: 227–241. doi:10.1080/10236244.2010.501135.
[80] Mariani, Stefano. 2006. Life-history- and ecosystem-driven variation in composition and residence pattern of seabream species (Perciformes: Sparidae) in two Mediterranean coastal lagoons. Marine Pollution Bulletin 53: 121–127. doi:10.1016/j.marpolbul.2005.09.019.
[81] Zohar, Yonathan, M. Harel, S. Hassin, and Amos Tandler. 1995. Gilt-head sea bream (Sparus aurata). In Broodstock Management and Egg and Larval Quality, ed. Niall R. Bromage and Ronald J. Roberts, 94–117. Wiley.
[82] Colloca, F., and S. Cerasi. 2005. Cultured Aquatic Species Information Programme. Sparus aurata. Rome: FAO Fisheries and Aquaculture Department.
[83] Brown, Richard Cameron. 2003. Genetic Management and Selective Breeding in Farmed Populations of Gilthead Seabream (Sparus aurata). Thesis or Dissertation, University of Stirling.
[84] Ferraresso, Serena, Nicola Vitulo, Alba N. Mininni, Chiara Romualdi, Barbara Cardazzo, Enrico Negrisolo, Richard Reinhardt, Adelino VM Canario, Tomaso Patarnello, and Luca Bargelloni. 2008. Development and validation of a gene expression oligo microarray for the gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata). BMC Genomics 9: 580. doi:10.1186/1471-2164-9-580.
[85] Andrades, J. A., J. Becerra, and P. Fernández-Llebrez. 1996. Skeletal deformities in larval, juvenile and adult stages of cultured gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.). Aquaculture 141: 1–11. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(95)01226-5.
[86] Faustino, M., and D. M. Power. 2001. Osteologic development of the viscerocranial skeleton in sea bream: alternative ossification strategies in teleost fish. Journal of Fish Biology 58: 537–572. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2001.tb02272.x.
[87] Elbal, M. T, M. P Garcı́a Hernández, M. T Lozano, and B Agulleiro. 2004. Development of the digestive tract of gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.). Light and electron microscopic studies. Aquaculture 234: 215–238. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2003.11.028.
[88] Russo, T., C. Costa, and S. Cataudella. 2007. Correspondence between shape and feeding habit changes throughout ontogeny of gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata L., 1758. Journal of Fish Biology 71: 629–656. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01528.x.
[89] Parra, G, and M Yúfera. 2000. Feeding, physiology and growth responses in first-feeding gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata L.) larvae in relation to prey density. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 243: 1–15. doi:10.1016/S0022-0981(99)00106-9.
[90] Pawson, M.G., and G.D. Pickett. 1996. The Annual Pattern of Condition and Maturity in Bass, Dicentrarchus Labrax, in Waters Around England and Wales. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 76: 107. doi:10.1017/S0025315400029040.
[91] Brusléa-Sicard, S., and B. Fourcault. 1997. Recognition of sex-inverting protandric Sparus aurata: ultrastructural aspects. Journal of Fish Biology 50: 1094–1103. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1997.tb01633.x.
[92] Mehanna, Sahar Fahmy. 2007. A preliminary assessment and management of gilthead bream Sparus aurata in the Port Said fishery, the Southeastern Mediterranean, Egypt. Turkish Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 7: 123–130.
[93] Kadmon, G., Z. Yaron, and H. Gordin. 1985. Sequence of gonadal events and oestradiol levels in Sparus aurata (L.) under two photoperiod regimes. Journal of Fish Biology 26: 609–620. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1985.tb04301.x.
[94] Wassef, Elham A. 1990. Growth rate of gilthead bream Sparus aurata L. J. K. A. U. Mar. Sci. 1: 55–65.
[95] Gonçalves, J. M. S., L. Bentes, P. G. Lino, J. Ribeiro, A. V. M. Canário, and K. Erzini. 1997. Weight-length relationships for selected fish species of the small-scale demersal fisheries of the south and south-west coast of Portugal. Fisheries Research 30: 253–256. doi:10.1016/S0165-7836(96)00569-3.
[96] Zohar, Y., M. Abraham, and H. Gordin. 1978. The gonadal cycle of the captivity-reared hermaphroditic teleost Sparus aurata (L.) during the first two years of life. Annales de Biologie Animale Biochimie Biophysique 18: 877–882. doi:10.1051/rnd:19780519.
[97] Wong, Ten-Tsao, Shigeho Ijiri, and Yonathan Zohar. 2006. Molecular Biology of Ovarian Aromatase in Sex Reversal: Complementary DNA and 5′-Flanking Region Isolation and Differential Expression of Ovarian Aromatase in the Gilthead Seabream (Sparus aurata). Biology of Reproduction 74: 857–864. doi:10.1095/biolreprod.105.045351.
[98] Navarro, Ana, María J. Zamorano, Silvia Hildebrandt, Rafael Ginés, Cristóbal Aguilera, and Juan M. Afonso. 2009. Estimates of heritabilities and genetic correlations for growth and carcass traits in gilthead seabream (Sparus auratus L.), under industrial conditions. Aquaculture 289: 225–230. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2008.12.024.
[99] Addis, Maria Filippa, Roberto Cappuccinelli, Vittorio Tedde, Daniela Pagnozzi, Maria Cristina Porcu, Elia Bonaglini, Tonina Roggio, and Sergio Uzzau. 2010. Proteomic analysis of muscle tissue from gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata, L.) farmed in offshore floating cages. Aquaculture 309: 245–252. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2010.08.022.
[100] Boglione, Clara, Flavio Gagliardi, Michele Scardi, and Stefano Cataudella. 2001. Skeletal descriptors and quality assessment in larvae and post-larvae of wild-caught and hatchery-reared gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L. 1758). Aquaculture 192: 1–22. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(00)00446-4.
[101] Carrillo, J, G Koumoundouros, P Divanach, and J Martinez. 2001. Morphological malformations of the lateral line in reared gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L. 1758). Aquaculture 192: 281–290. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(00)00454-3.
[102] Paperna, I. 1978. Swimbladder and skeletal deformations in hatchery bred Spams aurata. Journal of Fish Biology 12: 109–114. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1978.tb04157.x.
[103] Koumoundouros, G., G. Oran, P. Divanach, S. Stefanakis, and M. Kentouri. 1997. The opercular complex deformity in intensive gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.) larviculture. Moment of apparition and description. Aquaculture 156: 165–177. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(97)89294-0.
[104] Afonso, J. M., D. Montero, L. Robaina, N. Astorga, M. S. Izquierdo, and R. Ginés. 2000. Association of a lordosis-scoliosis-kyphosis deformity in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata) with family structure. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 22: 159–163. doi:10.1023/A:1007811702624.
[105] Castro, Jaime, Ania Pino-Querido, Miguel Hermida, David Chavarrías, Roberto Romero, Luis A. García-Cortés, Miguel A. Toro, and Paulino Martínez. 2008. Heritability of skeleton abnormalities (lordosis, lack of operculum) in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata) supported by microsatellite family data. Aquaculture 279: 18–22. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2008.04.023.
[106] Balebona, M. C., M. A. Morinigo, J. A. Andrades, J. A. Santamaria, J. Becerra, and J. S. Borrego. 1993. Microbiological study of gilthead sea bream S. aurata L. affected by lordosis a skeletal deformity. Bull. Eur. Assoc. Fish Pathol. 13: 33.
[107] Chatain, Béatrice. 1994. Abnormal swimbladder development and lordosis in sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax) and sea bream (Sparus auratus). Aquaculture 119: 371–379. doi:10.1016/0044-8486(94)90301-8.
[108] Reyes-Tomassini, Jose J. 2009. Behavioral and Neuroendocrine Correlates of Sex Change in the Gilthead Seabream (Sparus aurata). University of Maryland.
[109] Gordin, H., and Y. Zohar. 1978. Induced spawning of Sparus aurata (L.) by means of hormonal treatments. Annales de Biologie Animale Biochimie Biophysique 18: 985–990. doi:10.1051/rnd:19780535.
[110] Liarte, Sergio, Elena Chaves-Pozo, Alicia García-Alcazar, Victoriano Mulero, José Meseguer, and Alfonsa García-Ayala. 2007. Testicular involution prior to sex change in gilthead seabream is characterized by a decrease in DMRT1 gene expression and by massive leukocyte infiltration. Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology 5: 20. doi:10.1186/1477-7827-5-20.
[111] Barbaro, A., A. Francescon, G. Bozzato, A. Merlin, P. Belvedere, and L. Colombo. 1997. Induction of spawning in gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata L., by a long-acting GnRH agonist and its effects on egg quality and daily timing of spawning. Aquaculture 154: 349–359. doi:10.1016/S0044-8486(97)00067-7.
[112] Gorshkov, S., H. Gordin, G. Gorshkova, and W. Knibb. 1997. Reproductive constraints for family selection of the gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata L.). Israeli Journal of Aquaculture 49: 124–134. CABDirect2.
[113] Meiri, Iris, Yoav Gothilf, Yonathan Zohar, and Abigail Elizur. 2002. Physiological changes in the spawning gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata, succeeding the removal of males. Journal of Experimental Zoology 292: 555–564. doi:10.1002/jez.10072.
[114] Zohar, Y., and H. Gordin. 1979. Spawning kinetics in the gilthead sea-bream, Sparus aurata L. after low doses of human chronic gonadotropin. Journal of Fish Biology 15: 665–670. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1979.tb03675.x.
[115] Saavedra, Margarida, and Pedro Pousão-Ferreira. 2006. A preliminary study on the effect of lunar cycles on the spawning behaviour of the gilt-head sea bream, Sparus aurata. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 86: 899–901. doi:10.1017/S0025315406013841.
[116] Zohar, Y., A. Goren, M. Tosky, G. Pagelson, D. Leibovitz, and Y. Koch. 1989. The bioactivity of gonadotropin releasing hormones and its regulation in the gilthead seabream,Sparus aurata: in vivo andin vitro studies. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 7: 59–67. doi:10.1007/BF00004690.
[117] Lasserre, G., and P. J. Labourg. 1974. Comparison of growth of Sparus auratus L. in regions of Arcachon and Sete (2nd note). Vie et Milieu Serie A - Biologie Marine 24: 357–363.
[118] Kolkovski, S., A. Arieli, and A. Tandler. 1997. Visual and chemical cues stimulate microdiet ingestion in sea bream larvae. Aquaculture International 5: 527–536. doi:10.1023/A:1018305416501.
[119] Hubbard, P. C., E. N. Barata, and A. V. M. Canário. 2003. Olfactory Sensitivity to Catecholamines and their Metabolites in the Goldfish. Chemical Senses 28: 207–218. doi:10.1093/chemse/28.3.207.
[120] Brown, Culum. 2014. Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal Cognition 18: 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0.
[121] Amundsen, Lasse, and Martin Landro. 2011. Marine seismic sources part VIII: Fish hear a great deal. Recent Advances in Technology 8: 1–5.
[122] Buscaino, Giuseppa, Francesco Filiciotto, Gaspare Buffa, Antonio Bellante, Vincenzo Di Stefano, Anna Assenza, Francesco Fazio, Giovanni Caola, and Salvatore Mazzola. 2010. Impact of an acoustic stimulus on the motility and blood parameters of European sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax L.) and gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.). Marine Environmental Research 69: 136–142. doi:10.1016/j.marenvres.2009.09.004.
[123] Filiciotto, Francesco, Vincenzo Maximiliano Giacalone, Francesco Fazio, Gaspare Buffa, Giuseppe Piccione, Vincenzo Maccarrone, Vincenzo Di Stefano, Salvatore Mazzola, and Giuseppa Buscaino. 2013. Effect of acoustic environment on gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata): Sea and onshore aquaculture background noise. Aquaculture 414–415: 36–45. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2013.07.042.
[124] Dijkgraaf, S. 1967. Biological significance of the lateral line organs. In Lateral Line Detectors: Proceedings. Edited by Phyllis H. Cahn, ed. Phyllis H. Cahn, 83–95. Indiana University Press.
[125] Bleckmann, Horst. 1986. Role of the Lateral Line in Fish Behaviour. In The Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, ed. Tony J. Pitcher, 177–202. Springer US.
[126] Suckling, J. A. 1967. Trunk  lateral  line  nerves:  some  anatomical aspects. In Lateral Line Detectors: Proceedings., ed. Phyllis H. Cahn, 45–52. Indiana University Press.
[127] Fischer, W. 1987. Sparidae. In Guide Fao d’Identification des Espèces pour les Besoins de la Pêche. Méditerranée et Mer Noir - Zone de Pêche 37. Vertébrés, ed. W. Fischer, M. Schneider, and M. L. Bauchot. Vol. 2. Rome: FAO.
[128] Bagni, M., C. Civitareale, A. Priori, A. Ballerini, M. Finoia, G. Brambilla, and G. Marino. 2007. Pre-slaughter crowding stress and killing procedures affecting quality and welfare in sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax) and sea bream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 263: 52–60. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2006.07.049.
[129] Tort, L., J. O. Sunyer, E. Gómez, and A. Molinero. 1996. Crowding stress induces changes in serum haemolytic and agglutinating activity in the gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata. Veterinary Immunology and Immunopathology 51: 179–188. doi:10.1016/0165-2427(95)05502-9.
[130] Arends, R. J., J. M. Mancera, J. L. Munoz, SE Wendelaar Bonga, and G. Flik. 1999. The stress response of the gilthead sea bream (Sparus aurata L.) to air exposure and confinement. Journal of Endocrinology 163: 149–157. doi:10.1677/joe.0.1630149.
[131] Montero, D., M. S. Izquierdo, L. Tort, L. Robaina, and J. M. Vergara. 1999. High stocking density produces crowding stress altering some physiological and biochemical parameters in gilthead seabream, Sparus aurata, juveniles. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 20: 53–60. doi:10.1023/A:1007719928905.
[132] Rotllant, J., P. H. M. Balm, J. Pérez-Sánchez, S. E. Wendelaar-Bonga, and L. Tort. 2001. Pituitary and Interrenal Function in Gilthead Sea Bream (Sparus aurata L., Teleostei) after Handling and Confinement Stress. General and Comparative Endocrinology 121: 333–342. doi:10.1006/gcen.2001.7604.
[133] Ortuño, J., M. A. Esteban, and J. Meseguer. 2001. Effects of short-term crowding stress on the gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata L.) innate immune response. Fish Shellfish Immunology 11: 187–197. doi:10.1006/fsim.2000.0304.
[134] Matos, Elisabete, Amparo Gonçalves, Maria Leonor Nunes, Maria Teresa Dinis, and Jorge Dias. 2010. Effect of harvesting stress and slaughter conditions on selected flesh quality criteria of gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Aquaculture 305: 66–72. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2010.04.020.
[135] Goldan, O. 1992. The control of growth depensation in juvenile gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata. Dissertation, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem.
[136] Réale, Denis, Simon M. Reader, Daniel Sol, Peter T. McDougall, and Niels J. Dingemanse. 2007. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82: 291–318. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x.
[137] Robb, D H F, and S C Kestin. 2002. Methods Used to Kill Fish: Field Observations and Literature Reviewed. Animal Welfare 11: 269–282.
[138] Van De Vis, Hans, Steve Kestin, David Robb, Jörg Oehlenschläger, Bert Lambooij, Werner Münkner, Holmer Kuhlmann, et al. 2003. Is humane slaughter of fish possible for industry? Aquaculture Research 34: 211–220. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2109.2003.00804.x.